Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue HP Branigan, MJ Pickering, AA Cleland Cognition 75 (2), B13-B25, 2000 | 1443 | 2000 |
The use of lexical and syntactic information in language production: Evidence from the priming of noun-phrase structure AA Cleland, MJ Pickering Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2), 214-230, 2003 | 646 | 2003 |
Syntactic priming in written production: Evidence for rapid decay HP Branigan, MJ Pickering, AA Cleland Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 6, 635-640, 1999 | 334 | 1999 |
Syntactic alignment and participant role in dialogue HP Branigan, MJ Pickering, JF McLean, AA Cleland Cognition 104 (2), 163-197, 2007 | 270 | 2007 |
Do writing and speaking employ the same syntactic representations? AA Cleland, MJ Pickering Journal of Memory and Language 54 (2), 185-198, 2006 | 232 | 2006 |
Activation of syntactic information during language production MJ Pickering, HP Branigan, AA Cleland, AJ Stewart Journal of psycholinguistic research 29, 205-216, 2000 | 99 | 2000 |
Sex differences in the spatial representation of number. R Bull, AA Cleland, T Mitchell Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 142 (1), 181, 2013 | 98 | 2013 |
Frequency effects in spoken and visual word recognition: Evidence from dual-task methodologies. AA Cleland, MG Gaskell, PT Quinlan, J Tamminen Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 32 (1), 104, 2006 | 73 | 2006 |
Familiarity breeds dissent: Reliability analyses for British-English idioms on measures of familiarity, meaning, literality, and decomposability E Nordmann, AA Cleland, R Bull Acta Psychologica 149, 87-95, 2014 | 48 | 2014 |
The role of numerical and non-numerical cues in nonsymbolic number processing: Evidence from the line bisection task AA Cleland, R Bull Quarterly journal of experimental psychology 68 (9), 1844-1859, 2015 | 44 | 2015 |
The influence of sentential position on noun phrase structure priming A Melinger, AA Cleland Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 64 (11), 2211-2235, 2011 | 40 | 2011 |
Implicit response-irrelevant number information triggers the SNARC effect: Evidence using a neural overlap paradigm T Mitchell, R Bull, AA Cleland Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 65 (10), 1945-1961, 2012 | 38 | 2012 |
The nature of phoneme representation in spoken word recognition. MG Gaskell, PT Quinlan, J Tamminen, AA Cleland Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 137 (2), 282, 2008 | 37 | 2008 |
Polysemy advantage with abstract but not concrete words B Jager, AA Cleland Journal of psycholinguistic research 45 (1), 143-156, 2016 | 25 | 2016 |
Participant role and syntactic alignment in dialogue HP Branigan, MJ Pickering, JF McLean, AA Cleland Cognition 104, 163-197, 2007 | 25 | 2007 |
Processing semantic ambiguity: Different loci for meanings and senses J Tamminen, AA Cleland, PT Quinlan, MG Gaskell Proceedings of the 28th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society …, 2006 | 20 | 2006 |
Polysemy in the mental lexicon: relatedness and frequency affect representational overlap B Jager, MJ Green, AA Cleland Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 31 (3), 425-429, 2016 | 16 | 2016 |
Non-symbolic numerosities do not automatically activate spatial–numerical associations: Evidence from the SNARC effect AA Cleland, K Corsico, K White, R Bull Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 73 (2), 295-308, 2019 | 15 | 2019 |
Automaticity of access to numerical magnitude and its spatial associations: The role of task and number representation. AA Cleland, R Bull Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 45 (2), 333, 2019 | 14 | 2019 |
Spoken word processing creates a lexical bottleneck AA Cleland, J Tamminen, PT Quinlan, MG Gaskell Language and Cognitive Processes 27 (4), 572-593, 2012 | 14 | 2012 |