M Thomas, A Karmiloff-Smith - Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2002 - cambridge.org
It is often assumed that similar domain-specific behavioural impairments found in cases of adult brain damage and developmental disorders correspond to similar underlying causes …
S Pinker - WW Norton&Company, 1997 - reasonpapers.com
The medieval curriculum comprised seven liberal arts, divided into the lower-level trivium (grammar, logic, and rhetoric) and the upper-level quadrivium (geometry, astronomy …
Cognitive science approaches the study of mind and intelligence from an interdisciplinary perspective, working at the intersection of philosophy, psychology, artificial intelligence …
Are morphological patterns learned in the form of rules? Some models deny this, attributing all morphology to analogical mechanisms. The dual mechanism model (Pinker, S., & Prince …
MT Ullman, S Corkin, M Coppola… - Journal of cognitive …, 1997 - ieeexplore.ieee.org
Language comprises a lexicon for storing words and a grammar for generating rule- governed forms. Evidence is presented that the lexicon is part of a temporal-parietalhnedial …
SD Epstein, S Flynn, G Martohardjono - Behavioral and Brain …, 1996 - cambridge.org
To what extent, if any, does Universal Grammar (UG) constrain second language (L2) acquisition? This is not only an empirical question, but one which is currently investigable. In …
GF Marcus, U Brinkmann, H Clahsen, R Wiese… - Cognitive …, 1995 - Elsevier
Language is often explained as the product of generative rules and a memorized lexicon. For example, most English verbs take a regular past tense suffix (ask-asked), which is …
This book is a definitive reference source for the growing, increasingly more important, and interdisciplinary field of computational cognitive modeling, that is, computational psychology …
Learning the English past tense is characterized by a U-shaped learning function for the irregular verbs. Existing cognitive models often rely on a sudden increase in vocabulary, a …